- Research
- Open access
- Published:
The buffering effect of tangible social support on financial stress: influence on psychological well-being and psychosomatic symptoms in a large sample of the adult general population
International Journal for Equity in Health volume 13, Article number: 85 (2014)
Abstract
Introduction
Financial stress is an important source of distress and is related to poor mental and physical health outcomes. The present study investigated whether tangible social support could buffer the effect of financial stress on psychological and psychosomatic health.
Methods
Two separate postal surveys were sent to random samples in five counties in Sweden in 2004 and 2008, with a total of 84 263 respondents. The questionnaires included questions about financial stress, tangible social support, psychosomatic symptoms, and psychological well-being (General Health Questionnaire-12).
Results
Individuals with high financial stress and low tangible social support had six to seven times increased odds ratios for low psychological well-being and many psychosomatic symptoms. By contrast, individuals with high financial stress and high tangible social support had only two to three times increased odds ratios for low psychological well-being and three to four times increased odds ratios for many psychosomatic symptoms, suggesting a buffering effect of tangible social support. Consistent with the buffering hypothesis, there were significant interactions between financial stress and social support, particularly in relation to low psychological well-being.
Conclusions
Social support had its strongest effect at high levels of financial stress. The question whether the altering of our social networks may improve physical health is important for the prevention of ill health in people experiencing financial stress. Strengthening social networks may have the potential to influence health-care costs and improve quality of life.
Background
Financial stress, the persistent inability to afford the basic necessities of life, is an important source of distress and is related to poor mental and physical health outcomes in the population [1]-[4]. Financial stress has been suggested to be a powerful independent predictor for both the onset and persistence of episodes of mental disorders [5]. A common health-promoting factor often discussed in relation to chronic life strain is social support. It refers to the material and emotional resources that are available to a person through interpersonal contacts. It is associated with a reduced risk of a wide range of health outcomes including mental health [6]-[9], physical health [6,7,9]-[11], and mortality [6,9,12,13].
Social support can be assessed by both community-centric and person-centric instruments. There is an ongoing debate about whether social support enhances health irrespective of the level of stress (main or direct effect) or because social support buffers, or moderates, the effects of stress (interactive effect) [14]-[16]. The existence of a buffering effect requires a statistically significant interaction between the measures of social support and stress [16]. Stress-buffering effects have been observed inconsistently [15,17], whereas the main effect of social support on health has generally been replicated [18]. It has been suggested that the buffering effect on a specific life stress is observed only for social support aimed at alleviating the specific stress [14,15] and that tangible social support is the most probable aspect of social support that can buffer the effects of financial stress [14]. Peirce et al. suggested that research on social support and health should focus on specific dimensions of perceived support in combination with carefully matched types of life stress [19]. Although many studies have investigated the buffering effect of different aspects of social support on general life stress, few studies have focused on the relationship between financial stress and tangible social support in the context of the suggestions mentioned above.
The present study followed these suggestions by investigating the buffering effect of tangible social support on financial stress in relation to low psychological well-being and psychosomatic symptoms. The hypothesis was that high tangible social support would buffer the effects of financial stress on psychological and psychosomatic health. By investigating these specific associations in a large (N > 80 000) sample of the adult general population, the study contributes important new information to the research field.
Methods
Study design, population and procedure
This was a cross-sectional study. Postal surveys are distributed every four years by the county councils in the five Swedish counties of Uppsala, Sörmland, Västmanland, Värmland, and Örebro, in order to monitor the psychosocial health of the population. In total, the five counties comprise about 1 400 000 inhabitants. The present study involves data from one survey that was distributed during September to November 2004, and one survey that was distributed during March to May 2008. Random samples of 68 460 people for the 2004 survey and 68 710 for the 2008 survey, aged 18-84 years and stratified by sex, age, and city (and parts of the city for larger cities), were drawn from the total population by Statistics Sweden. They were asked to participate in each study by completing a postal survey questionnaire supplied with a prepaid return envelope. After 10days, a reminder was sent to the participants who had not responded. Ten days after the first reminder, a new questionnaire was sent together with a second reminder. A new questionnaire together with a third reminder was sent four weeks after the first reminder. The questionnaires were scanned and transformed into a data file with no personal identification of the participants.
The study followed the Swedish guidelines for studies of social sciences and humanities according to the Declaration of Helsinki. According to Swedish law (Ethical Review Act 2003:460), this type of anonymous study does not require ethical approval by a medical faculty.
Measures
Demographic data
Sex was categorised as (1) male and (2) female. Age was categorised as (1) 18-24 years, (2) 25-34 years, (3) 35-44 years, (4) 45-54 years, (5) 55-64 years, (6) 65-74 years, and (7) 75-84 years. Employment status was categorised as (1) employed, (2) self-employed, (3) on parental leave, (4) on leave, other reasons (5) full-time housewife/husband, (6) student, (7) unemployed, (8) on sick leave or early retirement, (9) old-age pension, and (10) other. Information about country of birth was obtained for each participant from Statistics Sweden and classified as (1) Sweden, (2) other country in Scandinavia, and (3) country outside of Scandinavia.
Year of survey
Societal factors such as trade conditions and unemployment rates might have changed between the two surveys. Such societal factors might be associated with both financial stress and ill health and could affect the results. For example, it has been shown that the association between unemployment and mortality weakens as the general unemployment rate increases [20]. Therefore, we adjusted all analyses for year of survey.
Psychosomatic symptoms
The questionnaire asked the question "How often during the past three months have you experienced the following symptoms: (i) pain in the shoulders/neck; (ii) pain in the back/hips; (iii) pain in the hands/arms/legs/knees/feet; (iv) abdominal pain; (v) headache/migraine; (vi) anxiety/nervousness; (vii) feelings of fatigue/feebleness; (viii) sleeping problems; (ix) depression; (x) dizziness; or (xi) irritated mucous membranes?" The responses options were: 0, never; 1, rarely; 2, several times; and 3, most of the time. The internal consistency of the questions of psychosomatic symptoms was α = 0.858. A summation index was created with a range of 0-36 points. The index was divided by the SD and was dichotomised with +1 SD as the cut-off point for having many psychosomatic symptoms.
Psychological well-being
The short version of the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12) was used. In this study, the Goldberg GHQ scoring method was applied [21], in which the responders can score 0-0-1-1 to give a total sum of 0-12 points. The internal consistency of the GHQ-12 items was α = 0.913. A summation index was created; for dichotomisation, a score of ≥3 was categorised as reduced psychological well-being [22].
Other chronic disease
The participants were asked: "Have you experienced any long-lasting disease (>6months), any persistent symptoms following an accident, any disability, or any other long-lasting health problem?". The response options were: 1, no; and 2, yes.
Financial stress
This measure was adapted according to previous measurements of financial stress [23], using the following two questions.
1. "During the past 12months, has it been difficult for you to pay your monthly costs, such as rent, mortgage, etc.?" The response options were: no; yes, one month; yes, 2months; yes, 3-5 months; and yes, 6-12 months. No economic difficulties was scored as 0 points, and economic difficulties for ≥1month was scored as 1 point.
2. "If you suddenly ended up in an unforeseen situation where you had to raise SEK 20 000 (~ 2100), would you be able to?" The response options were: yes (0 points) or no (1 point).
The variable financial stress was created by summing the points for questions 1 and 2, and then categorising the result as: no financial stress, 0 points (no difficulties with monthly costs and able to raise money); medium financial stress, 1 point (either difficulties with monthly costs or unable to raise money); and high financial stress, 2 points (difficulties with monthly costs and unable to raise money).
Tangible social support
This measure was adapted according to previous measurements of tangible or instrumental social support [24,25]. Tangible social support was measured by three questions as follows. "Do you have persons around you who would give you support in the event of personal problems or crises?" "Do you have persons around you who would help you with grocery shopping/cooking if you should fall ill?" "Do you have persons around you who would help you if you were to move to another dwelling?" The response options were: 1, no; 2, probably not; 3, yes, probably; and 4, yes, definitely. The internal consistency of the questions on tangible social support was α= 0.769. A summation index was created with a range of 4-12 points. The index was divided by the SD and dichotomised with −1 SD as the cut-off point for having low tangible social support.
Operationalisation of financial stress - tangible social support model
We created a six-quadrant model by combining the three levels of financial stress (no financial stress, medium financial stress, and high financial stress) with the two levels of tangible social support (low and high tangible social support).
Statistical analyses
Missing data were addressed by complete case analysis. The potential confounding variables of age, country of birth, employment status, other chronic disease, and year of the survey were adjusted for in the analyses. Because of a large number of missing cases (about 13%), educational level was not included as a control variable in the analyses.
Sex differences in the dependent and independent variables were analysed by χ2 test. The six-quadrant model was analysed using two separate categorical binary logistic regression models adjusted for age, country of birth, employment status, chronic disease, and year of survey to investigate any associations with psychological well-being and psychosomatic symptoms. All analyses were separated by sex. Differences between the subgroups in the six-quadrant model in relation to the outcome variables were analysed by χ2 test. Interaction effects between financial stress and tangible social support in relation to reduced psychological well-being and psychosomatic symptoms were analysed by general linear model (GLM).
All statistical analyses were performed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences for Windows, version 20.0.
Results
In total, there were 84 263 respondents, of which 45 704 (54.2%) were women and 38 559 (45.8%) were men (p < 0.001). The response rates were 63.7% for the 2004 survey and 59.2% for the 2008 survey, giving a combined response rate 61.4%. The descriptive data for the response rates according to county, sex, age, education, country of birth, and educational level are described in detail in the articles by Nilsson et al. [26] for the 2004 survey and by Åslund et al. [27] for the 2008 survey. The mean ages were 52.8years (standard deviation, SD = 18.3) for the 2004 survey and 53.8years (SD = 17.9) for the 2008 survey.
As shown in Table11, financial stress was more common among women, and low tangible social support was more common among men. Compared with men, a higher percentage of women reported low psychological well-being and many psychosomatic symptoms.
In the group with high tangible social support, high financial stress increased the likelihood of low psychological well-being by nearly three times among men and by more than two times among women compared with no financial stress (Table22). By contrast, in the group with low tangible social support, high financial stress increased the likelihood of low psychological well-being by nearly seven times among men and by nearly six times among women compared with no financial stress and high tangible social support. In the group with high tangible social support, high financial stress increased the likelihood of many psychosomatic symptoms by four times in men and by nearly three times in women compared with no financial stress (Table33). By contrast, in the group with low tangible social support, high financial stress increased the likelihood of many psychosomatic symptoms by more than seven times in men and by more than five times in women compared with no financial stress and high tangible social support. The adjustment for educational level was not performed in the analyses because of the large number of missing cases for this variable. However, a re-run of the analyses of the data in Tables22 and 3 with the inclusion of educational level did not alter the results in any significant way, although the odds ratios (ORs) for each subgroup increased slightly in both models.
The GLM was adjusted for age, country of birth, employment status, other chronic disease, and year of survey. There were significant interaction effects between financial stress and tangible social support in relation to low psychological well-being; the strongest effect was in men (Table44). These findings suggest that the effect of tangible social support was stronger in the presence of higher financial stress, as also shown in Figure11. The interaction effects between financial stress and tangible social support in relation to psychosomatic symptoms were noticeably weaker in men and were not significant in women.
Discussion
The present study investigated the buffering effect of tangible social support on financial stress in relation to psychological well-being and psychosomatic symptoms in a large sample of the adult general population in Sweden. In the group with high tangible social support, high financial stress increased the likelihood of low psychological well-being by two to three times and the likelihood of many psychosomatic symptoms by three to four times. However, in the group with low tangible social support, high financial stress increased the likelihood of both low psychological well-being and many psychosomatic symptoms by six to seven times when compared with no financial stress and high tangible social support. The associations between financial stress, low tangible social support, and ill health were more prominent among men. Consistent with the buffering hypothesis, there were significant interactions between financial stress and tangible social support in relation to psychological well-being. However, the analyses of psychosomatic symptoms showed weaker interaction effects in men and no effects in women. These findings suggest that tangible social support has its strongest buffering effect on psychological well-being at high levels of financial stress. However, tangible social support has a weaker effect on the relationship between financial stress and psychosomatic symptoms, particularly in women. Financial stress had a substantial impact on emotional and physical well-being even when tangible social support was available.
The present findings partly support the suggestion that the buffering effect on a specific life stress is prominent only if the social support factor is aimed at alleviating the specific stress [14,15]. Similar buffering effects of tangible social support have been found in previous studies of financial stress in relation to psychological well-being [16] and alcohol consumption [19]. However, a study by Krause et al. investigating chronic financial strain in relation to depressive symptoms found no buffering effect of tangible social support [28]. That study focused on a population comprising solely elderly people (aged >65years). Although the present study involved a random sample of the population aged 18-84 years, the differences in response rates caused an imbalance in the data toward a preponderance of older participants. This suggests that differences in findings between the Krause et al. study and the present study might not be explained by population factors. Rather, the difference in outcome measures is the most plausible explanation, as the more severe condition of depression might be less influenced by tangible social support and is not comparable to the less severe, general measures of psychological well-being and psychosomatic symptoms in the present study.
The buffering effect of tangible social support may be explained by several possible mechanisms. The perception that others can and will provide necessary resources may redefine the potential for harm and prevent the situation from being appraised as highly stressful [14]. The reception of support signals that others care for and value the distressed person, boosting his or her sense of mattering and self-esteem [7]. The perception of available tangible support may thus intervene between the experience of financial stress and the pathological outcome by reducing the stress reaction and its harmful physiological processes. Tangible social support may also provide a solution to the problem associated with the stress factor by providing support and tangible help in the event of personal problems or crises, thereby reducing the stress reaction. Although the instrument used in the present study did not include direct questions about receiving financial assistance, the types of assistance described (whether the person could expect aid in personal crises, receive help with daily chores if they became sick, and receive help if they were moving) may be crucial to the easing or solving of financial problems.
The question of whether altering one´s social networks may improve physical health is very important [29]. From a health-care perspective, the prevention of ill health and promotion of good health may influence both health-care costs and quality of life. The answer to this question may provide important insights into and tools for pursuing basic social psychological questions, such as how the characteristics of our social networks influence our cognitive, behavioural, and physiological functions [29].
Limitations
There are several limitations in the present study. The overall response rate of 61.4% was not optimal. There were also differences in response rates between the subgroups within the sample; for example, males, younger individuals, those with a lower educational level, and individuals born outside Sweden all had lower response rates [26,27]. However, the statistical analyses controlled for potential confounding factors such as age, country of birth, employment status, other chronic disease, and year of survey. Because of the number of missing answers about educational level (about 13%), we chose to exclude it as a control variable from the analyses. However, a re-run of the analyses with educational level included revealed no major alterations of the results. Moreover, the imbalance in the data toward a preponderance of older participants may have influenced our results because it has been suggested that financial stress and social support may be particularly important for older people [28,29]. Our results may therefore be less generalisable to younger populations. Because of the anonymous study design, it was not possible to do a thorough non-response analysis to investigate this.
The cross-sectional design restricts the conclusions that can be drawn about cause and effect. Although our results show strong associations between financial stress, tangible social support, and ill health, the directions of the associations are unknown. Low psychological well-being and many psychosomatic symptoms might, for example, be related to higher rates of sick leave [30] or early retirement [31], life situations that are closely associated with lower income and higher risk of financial stress.
Moreover, the question in the financial stress measure regarding the ability to raise money did not specify between raising money from personal savings or from their support network, which could have created collinearity with the tangible support measure. Also, the social support question regarding ´persons around you who would give you support in the event of personal problems or crises´ may not necessarily be interpreted by the participants as getting tangible social support. However, we still deemed tangible social support to be the definition that best corresponded to the social support measure.
Another limitation regards the dichotomisation of the measures which might reduce the specificity of the data. These dichotomisations were necessary in order to create a balanced financial stress-tangible social support model, which was analysed by binary logistic regression. However, we complemented these analyses with a general linear model, using the non-dichotomised indices, which showed similar findings. The procedure with complementary statistical approaches can help to overcome shortcomings with the individual statistical methods and help to eliminate scaling artefacts.
Although we adjusted the analyses for confounding from chronic disease, we were not able to specify whether the chronic disease concerned a mental disorder or a physical disorder. It has been suggested that prior mental disorders can be a major risk factor for future stress generation [32,33]. Moreover, financial stress and ill health are related to several confounding demographic and psychosocial factors that were not controlled for in the present study and that might partly explain the findings, i.e., occupational class, income, marital status, long-term unemployment, and trade conditions in society. Since societal factors such as trade conditions and unemployment rates might have changed between the two surveys, this could have affected the results. Therefore, we adjusted all analyses for survey year.
However, the limitations may be balanced by the statistical power. Overall, there were 84 263 respondents, and the smallest subgroup of the financial stress - tangible social support model included 792 men and 996 women. These subgroup sizes equal the total population samples of many other studies and counteract the risk of random findings that accompany small sample designs. The strong associations between financial stress and tangible social support in relation to ill health are particularly interesting given that the study was set in Sweden, a highly egalitarian country with well-developed social security and social-welfare programs. Thus, poverty and financial stress might not be as devastating or life-threatening in Sweden as in countries with less developed social-welfare systems.
Conclusions
The question of whether altering our social networks may improve physical and mental health is important for the prevention of ill health, and the answer to this question may help reduce health-care costs and improve quality of life. Future research should focus on carefully defining the dimensions of social support that meet the demands of a specific life stressor, preferably in large generalisable population studies. The next step in the social support research field would be to use interdisciplinary methods to investigate further the physiological mechanisms behind the stress-buffering effects, such as the neuroendocrine and neurological pathways.
Authors´ contributions
Study concept and design: CÅ, BS, KWN. Acquisition of data: BS. Questionnaire management: BS. Analysis and interpretation of data: CÅ, KWN. Drafting of manuscript: CAcquisition of data: BS. Questionnaire management: BS. A, PL, KWN. Critical revision: CÅ, PL, BS, KWN. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Abbreviations
- GHQ-12:
-
General Health Questionnaire
- GLM:
-
General Linear Models
- SD:
-
Standard Deviation
- OR:
-
Odds Ratio
- CI:
-
Confidence Interval
References
Horwitz AV: The economy and social pathology. Ann Rev Sociol. 1984, 10: 95-119.
Marmot M: The Status Syndrome: How Social Standing Affects our Health and Longevity. 2004, Henry Holt, New York
Marmot M, Wilkinson RG: Social Determinants of Health. 2006, Oxford University Press, New York
Catalano R: The health effects of economic insecurity. Am J Public Health. 1991, 81 (9): 1148-1152.
Weich S, Lewis G: Poverty, unemployment and common mental disorders: population based cohort study. Br Med J. 1998, 317: 115-119.
Cobb S: Social support as a moderator of life stress. Psychosom Med. 1976, 38 (5): 300-314.
Thoits PA: Mechanisms linking social ties and support to physical and mental health. J Health Soc Behav. 2011, 52 (2): 145-161.
Kawachi I, Berkman LF: Social ties and mental health. J Urban Health. 2001, 78 (3): 458-467.
Umberson D, Montez JK: Social relationships and health: a flashpoint for health policy. J Health Soc Behav. 2010, 51: S54-S66.
Uchino BN, Cacioppo JT, Kiecolt-Glaser JK: The relationship between social support and physiological processes: a review with emphasis on underlying mechanisms and implications for health. Psychol Bull. 1996, 119 (3): 488-531.
Uchino BN: Social support and health: a review of physiological processes potentially underlying links to disease outcomes. J Behav Med. 2006, 29 (4): 377-387.
House JS, Landis KR, Umberson D: Social relationships and health. Science. 1988, 241: 540-545.
Reblin M, Uchino BN: Social and emotional support and its implications for health. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2008, 21: 201-205.
Cohen S, Wills TA: Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychol Bull. 1985, 98 (2): 310-357.
Lakey B, Orehek E: Relational Regulation Theory: a new approach to explain the link between perceived social support and mental health. Psychol Rev. 2011, 118 (3): 482-495.
Whelan CT: The role of social support in mediating the psychological consequences of economic stress. Sociol Health Illn. 1993, 15 (1): 86-101.
Alloway R, Bebbington P: The buffer theory of social support - a review of the literature. Psychol Med. 1987, 17: 91-108.
Finch JF, Okun MA, Pool GJ, Ruehlman LS: A comparison of the influence of conflictual and supportive social interactions on psychological distress. J Pers. 1999, 67 (4): 581-621.
Peirce RS, Frone MR, Russell M, Cooper ML: Financial stress, social support, and alcohol involvement: a longitudinal test of the buffering hypothesis in a general population survey. Health Psychol. 1996, 15 (1): 38-47.
Martikainen PT, Valkonen T: Excess mortality of unemployed men and women during a period of rapidly increasing unemployment. Lancet. 1996, 348 (9032): 909-912.
Goldberg DP, Gater R, Sartorius N, Ustun TB, Piccinelli M, Gureje O, Rutter C: The validity of two versions of the GHQ in the WHO study of mental illness in general health care. Psychol Med. 1997, 27 (1): 191-197.
Makowska Z, Merecz D, Moscicka A, Kolasa W: The validity of general health questionnaires, GHQ-12 and GHQ-28, in mental health studies of working people. Int J Occup Med Environ Health. 2002, 15 (4): 353-362.
Starrin B, Åslund C, Nilsson KW: Financial stress, shaming experiences and psychosocial ill-health: Studies into the finances-shame model. Soc Indicators Res. 2009, 91 (2): 283-298.
Zukewich N, Norris D: National Experiences and International Harmonization in Social Capital Measurement: A Beginning. 2005, Siena Group meeting, Helsinki
Franke S: Measurement of social capital: Reference document for public policy research, development, and evaluation. Policy Research Initiative (Canada). 2005
Nilsson KW, Leppert J, Simonsson B, Starrin B: Sence of coherence and psychological well-being: improvement with age. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2010, 64: 347-352.
Åslund C, Starrin B, Nilsson KW: Psychosomatic symptoms and low psychological well-being in relation to employment status: the influence of social capital in a large cross-sectional study in Sweden. Int J Equity Health. 2014, 13 (1): 22-
Krause N: Chronic financial strain, social support, and depressive symptoms among older adults. Psychol Aging. 1987, 2 (2): 185-192.
Cohen S, Janicki-Deverts D: Can we improve our physical health by altering our social networks?. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2009, 4 (4): 375-378.
Krantz G, Östergren P-O: Do common symptoms in women predict long spells of sickness absence? A prospective community-based study on Swedish women 40 to 50years of age. Scand J Public Health. 2002, 30: 176-183.
Härkäpää K: Psychosocial factors as predictors for early retirement in patients with chronic low back pain. J Psychosom Res. 1992, 36 (6): 553-559.
Hammen C: Stress and depression. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2005, 1: 293-319.
Daley SH, Hammen C, Burge D, Davila J, Paley B, Lindberg N, Herzberg DS: Predictors of the generation of episodic stress: a longitudinal study of late adolescent women. J Abnorm Psychol. 1997, 106 (2): 251-259.
Acknowledgements
Grants from the following funds and organisations are acknowledged. The Uppsala and Örebro Regional Research Council, Fredrik and Ingrid Thurings Foundation, the County Council of Västmanland, König-Söderströmska Foundation, The Swedish Psychiatric Foundation, and the Swedish Council for Working Life and Social Research (FAS). The sponsors of the study had no role in the study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, or writing of the report. We wish to thank the departments of community medicine of the county councils of Uppsala, Sörmland, Västmanland, Värmland, and Örebro for allowing us to use these data in the present study.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Authors’ original submitted files for images
Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.
Rights and permissions
This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly credited. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
About this article
Cite this article
Åslund, C., Larm, P., Starrin, B. et al. The buffering effect of tangible social support on financial stress: influence on psychological well-being and psychosomatic symptoms in a large sample of the adult general population. Int J Equity Health 13, 85 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12939-014-0085-3
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12939-014-0085-3