Skip to main content

Inequality in early childhood neurodevelopment in six poor rural counties of China: a decomposition analysis

Abstract

Background

Previous studies about inequality in children’s health focused more on physical health than the neurodevelopment. In this study, we aimed to evaluate the inequality in early childhood neurodevelopment in poor rural China and explore the contributions of socioeconomic factors to the inequality.

Method

Information of 2120 children aged 0 to 35 months and their households in six poor rural counties of China was collected during July – September, 2013. Age and Stages Questionnaire-Chinese version, concentration index and decomposition analysis were used to assess the neurodevelopment of early childhood, measure its inequality and evaluate the contributions of socioeconomic factors to the inequality, respectively.

Result

The prevalence of suspected developmental delay in children under 35 months of age in six poor rural counties of China was nearly 40%, with the concentration index of −0.0877. Household economic status, caregivers’ depressive symptoms, learning material and family support for learning were significantly associated with children’s suspected developmental delay, and explained 34.1, 14.1, 8.9 and 7.0% of the inequality in early childhood neurodevelopment, respectively.

Conclusion

The early childhood neurodevelopment in the surveyed area is poor and unfair. Factors including household economic status, caregivers’ depressive symptoms, learning material and family support for learning are significantly associated with children’s suspected developmental delay and early developmental inequality. The results highlight the urgent need of monitoring child neurodevelopment in poor rural areas. Interventions targeting the caregivers’ depressive symptoms, providing learning material and developmental appropriate stimulating activities may help improve early childhood neurodevelopment and reduce its inequality.

Background

Early childhood, marked by the most rapid development and the strongest plasticity, especially in the central nervous system, is the most crucial phrase throughout the lifespan [1]. Early childhood neurodevelopment includes the development in language, motor, problem solving and social-emotional domains, etc. The term developmental delay is frequently used to identify children with delay in one or more above domains [2]. Previous studies have shown that children with developmental delay would have higher risks of poor school performance, career development and mental health in the future [3,4,5,6]. Furthermore, the poor development in early life may also bring heavy psychological burden to their family [7]. For instance, Webster et al. [8] found that 42% of the parents of children with developmental delay had a clinically significant parenting stress. Cheng et al. [9] reported that 26.9% of mothers of children with cognitive delay had depressive symptoms. Nonetheless, previous experimental studies have also shown that children could better achieve their developmental potential if given adequate nutrition, cognitive stimulations and learning opportunities during their early childhood [10,11,12,13]. These studies provide a chance to improve developmental outcomes in early childhood.

It’s estimated that about 17 million children under 5 years of age in China failed to achieve their full developmental potential [14, 15]. Due to the poor sanitation, deficient healthcare, low maternal education, increased maternal depression and inadequate early stimulations, children in poor rural areas are more vulnerable to suppressed developmental potentials [14]. Moreover, compared with caregivers in urban areas, caregivers in poor rural areas may care less about their children’s neurodevelopment, resulting in late detection and interventions of children with developmental delay.

Health inequality is unnecessary and unfair difference in health observed in populations [16]. And it is an ethical imperative to ensure all people attain their health, irrespective of their social-economic status, race, gender, etc. [17]. Averages health status cannot provide a complete representation of changes in health in a population and it may conceal the true disparity among different populations [18]. So the reduction of health inequality is another important goal beyond the improvement of average health status.

Early childhood is a period that can exert determining influence on subsequent life. Therefore, promoting children’s health and reducing their health inequality becomes an imperative for all national and international communities. Studies focusing on inequality in children’s health show that there are some inequalities existing in children’s physical health, and that the main contributing factors of inequality in children’s physical health are living conditions, household economic status and maternal education [19,20,21,22,23,24]. However, study of inequality in children’s neurodevelopment are still limited [25], especially in China. So we conducted this study to evaluate early childhood neurodevelopment and its inequality in poor rural China, and to explore the contributions of socioeconomic factors to the inequality.

Methods

Study subjects

The data used in this study came from the baseline investigation of Integrated Early Childhood Development Project, a community-based intervention program, funded by United Nations International Children’s Emergency Fund (UNICEF). This baseline investigation was conducted from July, 2013 to September, 2013. Given the differences in culture, parenting behaviors and ethnic groups between North and South China, Shanxi Province in North China and Guizhou Province in South China were selected. Three counties in each province were randomly selected from the list of nationally designed concentrated poverty battlefields by National Health and Family Planning Commission (NHFPC) and UNICEF. A total of 83 villages were further selected from a total of 856 villages meeting the following criteria: 1) the number of children under 3 years of age should be more than 50 (10% of the villages could have less than 50 children), 2) the villages can be reached by motor vehicles, 3) the township should have health center and maternal and child health staffs. The average population size of these 83 villages in 2012 was 1719.6 ± 774.7. And the median of per capita income of these villages was 1500 Chinese Yuan (CNY) (inter-quartile range (IQR) 760 ~ 2300 CNY). All the households with children aged 0 to 35 months in these 83 villages were contacted by phone based on the information of children’s healthcare management system and informed to bring the children to the primary healthcare institution for investigation. According to the information of children’s healthcare management system, there were 4288 children aged 0 to 35 months in these 83 villages. But some children failed to participate in the investigation due to the migration and the inconvenient traffic in remote mountainous areas. Finally, 2953 children were surveyed, accounting for 68.9% of children registered in these areas. Among the 2953 children surveyed, 835 were excluded for missing information (183 for missing height or weight, 75 for missing developmental score, 575 for missing household income), leaving a total of 2120 children in this analysis.

Study instruments

Questionnaire

Face-to-face interviews were conducted with children’s caregivers by trained local healthcare providers using a standard structured questionnaire, which includes questions about the households, caregivers and children. All of the questions were derived from questionnaires of UNICEF’s 5th Multiple Indicator Cluster Survey (MICS5) [26].

Household per capita net income was used to reflect household economic status. Several questions about household income and cost of production were asked. The former includes household labor income, business income, transfer income and asset income. The latter is defined as the cost incurred to earn the income, including transport fee of migrant workers, material cost of business, and cost of seeds, fertilizers and pesticides for agricultural production, etc. Household net income was calculated as the difference between total income and total cost of production. Then household per capita net income was calculated as household net income / number of household members. Learning material and family support for learning were used to reflect the resources and stimulations children obtained. Children with picture books or toys were considered as having learning material. And according to the indicator list accompanying the questionnaires of MICS5 [26], children engaged four or more of the following activities with household members within 3 days before the survey were considered as having family support for learning: 1) reading books to the child, 2) telling stories to the child, 3) singing songs to or with the child, 4) taking child outside the home, 5) playing games with the child, 6) naming or counting things to or with the child.

Zung Self-rating Depression Scale (ZSDS)

ZSDS was used to assess the depressive symptoms of the caregivers. ZSDS is a 20-item self-reported questionnaire that is widely used as a screening tool, covering affective, psychological and somatic symptoms associated with depression [27]. There are four options for each item - a little of the time, some of the time, good part of the time, most of the time, and the corresponding responses are scored 1, 2, 3, 4 for the positively worded items and 4, 3, 2, 1 for the negatively worded items. An overall index score for the ZSDS is computed by summing each item’s score and dividing the sum by 0.8, resulting in a range of 25 to 100. Caregivers with index scores of 50 or more were identified as depression [28].

World Health Organization (WHO) child growth standards

Children’s height and weight were measured by two trained investigators during the study. The Z-scores of height-for-age and weight-for-height, calculated according to the WHO child growth standards published in 2006 [29], were used to evaluate children’s growth. Height-for-age Z-score less than −2 was defined as stunting, and weight-for-height Z-score less than −2 was defined as wasting.

Age and Stages Questionnaire (ASQ)

The ASQ-Chinese version was used to assess the early childhood neurodevelopment. The ASQ is a parent-completed developmental screening instrument [30]. The ASQ-third edition, which consists of 21 age-specific questionnaires intended for children ages 1–66 months, was published in 2009 and introduced to China later [31,32,33]. Each questionnaire consists of 30 items, covering five domains: communication, gross motor, fine motor, problem solving, and personal-social. Parents were asked to evaluate their children’s ability on every item, and their responses of “yes”, “sometimes” and “not yet” were scored 10, 5 and 0 points, respectively. The score of each domain was the sum of its corresponding six items’ scores. In this study, children were defined as suspected developmental delay (SDD) if the scores of one or more domains fell below the U.S. cut-off values.

All the variables used in this study are summarized in Table 1.

Table 1 Summary of all the variables in this study

Statistical analysis

The prevalence and the concentration index (C) of SDD were used to reflect the health status of children’s neurodevelopment and its inequality. Univariate and multivariate logistic regression analyses and odds ratio (OR) were used to assess the associations between socioeconomic factors and early childhood neurodevelopment. Univariate analyses were conducted first, and variables with p-value <0.2 were selected for the multivariate analyses. Decomposition analysis of concentration index was used to assess the contributions of socioeconomic factors to the inequality in early childhood neurodevelopment. In addition, we conducted two sensitivity analyses to test the robustness of our primary analyses. First, we imputed the missing income by the average of per capita income of their corresponding villages to test the impact of missing data. Second, we identified children with SDD by using cutoff points stemmed from our own data (10th and 15th percentile) to test the impact of different definitions of children’s SDD. Then the main analyses were repeated, including the concentration index of children’s SDD, the associations between potential factors and children’s SDD, and the contributions of these factors to the inequality in children’s SDD.

Statistical significance was defined P < 0.05 (for 2-side tests). All statistical analyses were performed with STATA, version 12.0. Figure 1 shows the framework of analyses in this study.

Fig. 1
figure 1

The framework of this study

Concentration index

The concentration index, which ranges from −1 to 1, provides a measure of the extent of inequality in health that is associated with economic status [34]. The index can be calculated by the following equation:

$$ C=\frac{2}{\mu}\mathit{\operatorname{cov}}\left({h}_i,{r}_i\right) $$
(1)

where h i is the health status of the i th individual, r i is the fractional rank of the i th individual in terms of household per capita net income and μ is the mean of the health status. A positive (negative) index suggests the health variable concentrated among the rich (poor). A value of zero suggests there is no inequality and a value of 0.2 to 0.3 is considered to be a high level of inequality, according to Health Inequality Monitoring, a book published by WHO [35].

Decomposition analysis of concentration index

The decomposition analysis of concentration index allows us to estimate the contributions of socioeconomic factors to the inequality in health [22]. Wagstaff et al. showed that for any linear regression model linking a variable of interest, y, to a set of k determinants, x k :

$$ y=\alpha +\sum {\beta}_k{x}_k+\varepsilon $$
(2)

where β k are the coefficients and ε is the error term. Given the relationship in Eq. (2), the concentration index for y, C, can be written as:

$$ C=\sum \left(\frac{\beta_k{\overline{x}}_k}{\mu}\right){C}_k+\frac{GC_{\varepsilon }}{\mu } $$
(3)

where μ is the mean of y, \( {\overline{x}}_k \) is the mean of x k , and C k is the concentration index for x k (defined analogously to C). In the last term, GC ε is a generalized concentration index for ε. The contribution rate (CR) of socioeconomic factor to inequality in y, can be written as:

$$ {CR}_{x_k}=\frac{\left({\beta}_k{\overline{x}}_k\right)/\mu }{C}{c}_k $$
(4)

For binary health variables, linear regression model like Eq. (2) is not appropriate. So marginal effects of covariates were calculated from binary logistic regression to replace β k in the decomposition analysis [18].

Results

Demographic information

Table 2 shows the demographic characteristics of the households, caregivers and children. Among the 2120 households, the median of household per capita net income was 2000 CNY (IQR 666.7 ~ 5000.0 CNY), and 50.8% were below the poverty line in China (2300 CNY). For caregivers, the mean age was 29.5 ± 9.2 years, 80.7% were mothers, 39.0% had depressive symptoms and 68.3% completed 9-year compulsory education, but only 14.4% completed the 12-year education of senior high school. Among the 2120 children, the mean age was 18.6 ± 9.6 months, 56.8% were boys, 89.8% had learning material and 85.8% had family support for learning. The prevalence of stunting and wasting was 13.6 and 3.4%, respectively.

Table 2 Demographic characteristic of household, caregiver and child

Prevalence and concentration index of SDD

Table 3 presents the prevalence and concentration index of SDD in children aged 0 to 35 months. The prevalence of SDD was 37.3%, and the prevalence of SDD in communication, gross motor, fine motor, problem solving and personal-social domain was 10.8, 17.6, 20.0, 17.5 and 17.5%, respectively. The concentration index of SDD in at least one of the domains was −0.0877, and the concentration indexes of SDD in communication, gross motor, fine motor, problem solving and personal-social domain were −0.1241, −0.1013, −0.1280, −0.1602 and −0.1304, respectively.

Table 3 Prevalence and concentration index of suspected developmental delay

Associations of SDD with socioeconomic factors

Table 4 presents associations of SSD with household, caregiver and child factors. Ten variables were identified potentially associated with SDD according to the univariate analysis and then selected for the multivariate logistic regression analysis. The multivariate regression results show that household per capita net income (OR: 0.97; 95% CI: 0.95, 0.99), the depressive symptoms of caregivers (OR: 2.30; 95% CI: 1.89, 2.81), the age of children (OR: 0.94; 95% CI: 0.93, 0.95), children with stunting (OR: 1.32; 95% CI: 1.01, 1.74), learning material (OR: 0.53; 95% CI: 0.38, 0.73) and family support for learning (OR: 0.67; 95% CI: 0.51, 0.87) were significantly associated with SDD.

Table 4 Associations of socioeconomic factors with suspected developmental delay

Contributions of socioeconomic factors to the inequality in SDD

The contributions of the socioeconomic factors to the inequality in SDD are presented in Table 5. The decomposition analysis of concentration index suggested that the ten factors included in the multivariate regression model explained 80.2% of the inequality in SDD, leading by household per capita net income (34.1%). Inequality in caregivers’ depressive symptoms, learning material and family support for learning explained 14.1, 8.9 and 7.0% of the inequality in SDD, respectively.

Table 5 Contributions of socioeconomic factors to inequality in suspected developmental delay

Sensitivity analyses

One sensitivity analysis was conducted by imputing data for missing income. The results are shown in Additional file 1: Tables S1–S3. Although the prevalence of SDD became slightly higher, the other main findings were still similar to the primary results, such as the medium-high level of inequality in children’ s SDD, the significant associations between household economic status, caregivers’ depressive symptoms, learning material, family support for learning and children’s SDD, and these factors’ main contributions to the inequality of children’s SDD.

Another sensitivity analysis was also done to test the impact of different definitions of children’ SDD. The results are shown in Additional file 1: Tables S4–S6. Almost all the main findings of the primary analyses were also confirmed in this sensitivity analysis.

Discussion

Our study revealed a high prevalence of SDD (37.4%) in six poor rural counties of China. It was much higher than the result of another Chinese study (27.1%) conducted in southern urban in children aged 6 to 30 months using ASQ-Chinese version [36], and one study (8.3%) conducted in Turkish children aged 3 to 36 months using ASQ-Turkish Version [37]. It is generally estimated the prevalence of confirmed developmental delay was about 10 to 20%, with significantly higher rates among children living in poverty [38]. On one hand, the high prevalence of SDD in our study might be a product of an imperfect measurement. It might be overestimated by using a Chinese-adapted screening tool but U.S. cutoff points [39]. On the other hand, the high prevalence might also result from the disadvantaged factors in poor rural areas and indicate an urgent need for early detection and interventions for children at risks of SDD. Therefore, there is an urgent need to incorporate the developmental monitoring into the existing well-child healthcare visits at primary care level to ensure timely identification and early interventions for children with SDD.

The concentration index of SDD ranged from −0.0877 to −0.1602 in our study, suggesting the existence of medium-high level of inequality in SDD, which concentrated among the poor, according to Health Inequality Monitoring by WHO and the study of Wagstaff [35, 40]. A study conducted in Indonesian also showed similar results in children aged 7 to 14 years [25]. Further studies are needed since the evidence of inequality in early childhood neurodevelopment is still limited.

Although household economic status was weakly associated with children’s SDD, it was the most important factor in explaining the inequality in children’s SDD in our study. Family is a key nurturing environment for children. Its economic status will affect children’s living conditions and their access to resources of nutrition, education and health. Previous studies have shown that poverty would severely limit children’s developmental potential [1, 14, 41,42,43]. For instance, poor areas might not be able to provide nutritious food, poverty could limit the availability of educational resources and the utilization of healthcare services for children. Furthermore, inequality in socioeconomic resources could result in inequality in ECD [1]. WHO states that “Any additional gain in social and economic recourses to a given family could result in commensurate gains in the development of the children in that family” [1]. Therefore, we suggest the government provide social and economic support to poor families, especially those with young children. For instance, micronutrient supplementation, age appropriate picture books and toys could be provided for children of poor families.

In our study, we found caregivers’ depressive symptoms were strongly associated with children’s SDD and the second leading factor in explaining the inequality in SDD. As we know, children’s early contact with outside environment is mostly mediated and controlled by caregivers. The daily interactions between caregivers and children constitute a main source of children’s early stimulations. Previous studies have shown that caregivers with depression would neglect children’s demands for nutrition, and emotional and cognitive stimulations [44,45,46,47]. Moreover, the adverse effects of maternal depression on the development of children are independent of the effects of poverty, malnutrition and other social disadvantages [48]. Identifying caregivers with depressive symptoms and taking timely, effective interventions may be one of the strategies to improve children’s neurodevelopment and reduce the inequality [49]. Thus, we recommend that it’s a priority to screen caregivers’ depressive symptoms, and this could be done during well-child visits by primary healthcare providers using a standardized screening tool.

We found learning material and family support for learning were significantly associated with children’s SDD, and explained 8.9 and 7.0% of the inequality in SDD, respectively. Previous studies have shown that effective early stimulations could boost children’s neurodevelopment [10, 13, 50, 51]. Effective investments in ECD also have the potential to reduce inequalities perpetuated by poverty, poor health, ect [52]. So it is recommended to implement ECD programs in poor areas, providing age-appropriate picture books, early learning opportunities to children and specific recommendations to caregivers about play, communication and parent-child interaction to improve children’s developmental outcomes and diminish the inequalities.

Although we didn’t find a statistically significant association between caregivers’ education and children’s SDD, we still included this factor into the decomposition analysis of concentration index of children’s SDD because previous studies have shown that caregivers’ education is an important factor for children’s development [49, 53]. Previous studies have shown that caregivers with lower education might have less healthcare knowledge, make less use of healthcare services, pay less attention to children’s development and have less decision making power to allocate family resources to children than caregivers with higher education [54,55,56,57]. What’s more, caregivers with lower education would have higher risks of developing depression [58]. The neglect of children’s demands for cognitive stimulations and the decrease of parent-child interactions caused by caregiver’s depression might negatively influence early childhood neurodevelopment [45, 46]. So, caregivers’ education might affect children’s development through these mechanisms, and then influence its inequality. In order to reduce the inequality in education as well as the inequality in offspring’s early development, we suggest further expanding the coverage of 9-year compulsory education in poor rural China and providing free high school education for poor children preferentially.

Studies about inequality in early childhood neurodevelopment are still limited, especially in China, so our study is of great significance for filling the gap. This study not only provided evidence on the general information of early childhood neurodevelopment and its inequality in poor rural China, but also investigated the contributions of socioeconomic factors to the inequality in early childhood development through the decomposition analysis of concentration index. In addition, sensitivity analyses were conducted to test the robustness of the results. The findings from the primary analyses and the sensitivity analyses both confirmed that the neurodevelopment of early childhood in the surveyed area is poor and unfair, and that some factors are significantly associated with children’s SDD and its inequality. The results of this study has great implication to combat the large number of children with developmental risks in China and other countries.

There are several limitations in this study. First, it is a cross-sectional study, which only allows us to identify the associations but not causal relationships. Further studies such as community intervention trial are needed to establish the causal relationships. Second, the representative of the sample is limited. The 83 villages in six counties of two provinces were selected according to certain criteria. Moreover, only 68.9% of the children registered in these villages were surveyed. Therefore, children surveyed in this study might not represent the entire population aged 0 to 35 months in the six poor rural counties of China. And cautions must be taken to generalize our results to other populations or areas. Third, the prevalence of SDD may be overestimated by using the U.S. cutoff points. One study showed that children younger than 24 months in China norm group scored significantly lower than their peers in U.S. norm group [39]. However, the findings of this study were robust in the sensitivity analyses. That may confirm the validity of the results of this study. Fourth, we used self-reported income to represent the economic status of the surveyed households, which would bring some measurement errors into the study. But previous studies have shown that the measurement error from self-reported earnings isn’t a big cause of concern in practice [59]. In addition, in order to measure income accurately as much as possible, we revised the questions of income sources carefully after pre-investigation to adapt to local conditions. Fifth, some subjects failed to provide complete information, especially household income, which might introduce information bias to our results. But the main findings in the sensitivity analyses about missing data were similar to the results in the primary analyses. So excluding the missing data might be acceptable in this regard. Sixth, we used the number of activities children engaged with their household members within 3 days before the survey to represent the level of family support for learning, which might bring some measurement errors in this study. In the future, standard tools need to be developed. Last but not the least, other factors that may associate with children’s neurodevelopment such as children’s birth order, birth weight, iron deficiency anemia and domestic violence were not analyzed in this study. Further studies are needed to clarify these associations.

Conclusion

The neurodevelopment of early childhood in the surveyed area is poor and unfair. The SDD was more prevalent in poor children. Factors including household economic status, caregivers’ depressive symptoms, learning material and family support for learning are significantly associated with children’s SDD and its inequality. The results highlight the urgent need of monitoring child neurodevelopment in poor rural areas. Interventions targeting the caregivers’ depressive symptoms, providing learning material and developmental appropriate stimulating activities may help improve early childhood neurodevelopment and reduce its inequality.

Abbreviations

ASQ:

Age and Stages Questionnaire

C:

Concentration index

CNY:

Chinese Yuan

ECD:

Early Childhood Development

IQR:

Inter-quartile range

MICS5:

5th Multiple Indicator Cluster Survey

NHFPC:

National Health and Family Planning Commission

OR:

Odds ratio

SDD:

Suspected developmental delay

UNICEF:

United Nations International Children’s Emergency Fund

WHO:

World Health Organization

ZSDS:

The Zung Self-rating Depression Scale

References

  1. Irwin LG, Siddiqi A, Hertzman C. Early child development: a powerful equalizer. Final report to the WHO Commission on social determinants of health. Geneva: WHO; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Petersen MC, Kube DA, Palmer FB. Classification of developmental delays. Semin Pediatr. Neuro. 1998;5:2–14.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Delgado CE, Vagi SJ, Scott KG, MacLean J, William E. Tracking preschool children with developmental delay: third grade outcomes. Am J Ment Retard. 2006;111:299–306.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Dornelas LF, Magalhães LC. Functional performance of school children diagnosed with developmental delay up to two years of age. Rev Paul Pediatr. 2016;34:78–85.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Fergusson DM, John Horwood L, Ridder EM. Show me the child at seven II: childhood intelligence and later outcomes in adolescence and young adulthood. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46:850–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Johnson W, Corley J, Starr JM, Deary IJ. Psychological and physical health at age 70 in the Lothian birth cohort 1936: links with early life IQ, SES, and current cognitive function and neighborhood environment. Health Psychol. 2011;30:1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Singer GH. Meta-analysis of comparative studies of depression in mothers of children with and without developmental disabilities. Am J Ment Retard. 2006;111:155–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Webster RI, Majnemer A, Platt RW, Shevell MI. Child health and parental stress in school-age children with a preschool diagnosis of developmental delay. J Child Neurol. 2008;23:32–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Cheng ER, Palta M, Poehlmann-Tynan J, Witt WP. The influence of children’s cognitive delay and behavior problems on maternal depression. J Pediatr. 2015;167:679–86.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Barros AJ, Matijasevich A, Santos IS, Halpern R. Child development in a birth cohort: effect of child stimulation is stronger in less educated mothers. Int J Epidemiol. 2010;39:285–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Wallander JL, Biasini FJ, Thorsten V, Dhaded SM, de Jong DM, Chomba E, et al. Dose of early intervention treatment during children’s first 36 months of life is associated with developmental outcomes: an observational cohort study in three low/low-middle income countries. BMC Pediatr. 2014;14:281.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Eickmann SH, Lima AC, Guerra MQ, Lima MC, Lira PI, Huttly SR, et al. Improved cognitive and motor development in a community-based intervention of psychosocial stimulation in northeast Brazil. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2003;45:536–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Walker SP, Chang SM, Powell CA. Grantham-McGregor SM. Effects of early childhood psychosocial stimulation and nutritional supplementation on cognition and education in growth-stunted Jamaican children: prospective cohort study. Lancet. 2005;366:1804–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Grantham-McGregor S, Cheung YB, Cueto S, Glewwe P, Richter L, Strupp B, et al. Developmental potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries. Lancet. 2007;369:60–70.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Lu C, Black MM, Richter LM. Risk of poor development in young children in low-income and middle-income countries: an estimation and analysis at the global, regional, and country level. Lancet Glob Health. 2016;4:e916–e22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Whitehead M. The concepts and principles of equity and health. Health Promot Int. 1991;6:217–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. CSDH. Closing the gap in a generation: health equity through action on the social determinants of health. Final report of the commission on social determinants of health. Geneva: WHO; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  18. O’donnell O, Van Doorslaer E, Wagstaff A, Lindelow M. Analyzing health equity using household survey data. Washington, DC: World Bank; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Zere E, McIntyre D. Inequities in under-five child malnutrition in South Africa. Int J Equity Health. 2003;2:7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Limwattananon S, Tangcharoensathien V, Prakongsai P. Equity in maternal and child health in Thailand. Bull World Health Organ. 2010;88:420–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Pei L, Wang D, Ren L, Yan H. Evaluation of the rural primary health care project on undernutrition equity among children in rural western China. Health Policy Plan. 2013;28:429–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Wagstaff A, Van Doorslaer E, Watanabe N. On decomposing the causes of health sector inequalities with an application to malnutrition inequalities in Vietnam. J Econ. 2003;112:207–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Goli S, Doshi R, Perianayagam A. Pathways of economic inequalities in maternal and child health in urban India: a decomposition analysis. PLoS One. 2013;8:e58573.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Chen L, Wu Y, Coyte PC. Income-related children’s health inequality and health achievement in China. Int J Equity Health. 2014;13:102.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Maika A, Mittinty MN, Brinkman S, Harper S, Satriawan E, Lynch JW. Changes in socioeconomic inequality in Indonesian children’s cognitive function from 2000 to 2007: a decomposition analysis. PLoS One. 2013;8:e78809.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. UNICEF. MICS tools: questionnaire and indicator list. Available from: http://mics.unicef.org/tools?round=mics5. Accessed 21 Feb 2017.

  27. Zung WW. A self-rating depression scale. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1965;12:63–70.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Biggs JT, Wylie LT, Ziegler VE. Validity of the Zung self-rating depression scale. Br J Psychiatry. 1978;132:381–5.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. World Health Organization. WHO child growth standards: length/height for age, weight-for-age, weight-for-length, weight-for-height and body mass index-for-age: methods and development. Geneva: WHO; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Squires J, Potter L, Bricker D. The ASQ user’s guide for the ages & stages questionnaires: a parent-completed, child-monitoring system. Baltimore: Paul H Brookes Publishing; 1995.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Squires J, Twombly E, Bricker D, Potter L. ASQ-3 User’s guide. 3rd ed. Baltimore: Paul H Brookes Publishing; 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Bian X, Yao G, Squires J, Wei M, Chen C, Fang B. Studies of the norm and psychometric properties of ages and stages questionnaires in shanghai children. Zhonghua Er Ke Za Zhi. 2010;48:492–6.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Yao G, Bian X, Squires J, Wei M, Song W. Cutoff scores of the ages and stages questionnaire-Chinese for screening infants and toddlers. Zhonghua Er Ke Za Zhi. 2010;48:824–8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Wagstaff A, Paci P, Van Doorslaer E. On the measurement of inequalities in health. Soc Sci Med. 1991;33:545–57.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. World Health Organization. Handbook on health inequality monitoring: with a special focus on low-and middle-income countries. Geneva: WHO; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Chen Y, Li J, Liu H. Analysis of 229 cases with ages & stages questionnaires and developmental screening test [article in Chinese]. Chin J Child Health Care. 2012;1:78–9.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Demirci A, Kartal M. The prevalence of developmental delay among children aged 3–60 months in Izmir, Turkey. Child Care Health Dev. 2016;42:213–9.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Singh A, Yeh CJ, Blanchard SB. Ages and stages questionnaire: a global screening scale. Bol Med Hosp Infant Mex. 2017;74:5–12.

  39. Miao Q, Chai Z, Squires J, Yan J, Chen J, Chen J, et al. Comparison in development of young children aged 1 to 66 months between China and U.S based on ages and stages questionnaire (the third edition). Chin J Evid Based Pediatr. 2017;12:111–5.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Wagstaff A. Socioeconomic inequalities in child mortality: comparisons across nine developing countries. Bull World Health Organ. 2000;78:19–29.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Duncan GJ, Brooks-Gunn J, Klebanov PK. Economic deprivation and early childhood development. Child Dev. 1994;65:296–318.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Duncan GJ, Ziol-Guest KM, Kalil A. Early-childhood poverty and adult attainment, behavior, and health. Child Dev. 2010;81:306–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Kiernan KE, Mensah FK. Poverty, maternal depression, family status and children’s cognitive and behavioural development in early childhood: a longitudinal study. J Soc Policy. 2009;38:569–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Rahman A, Iqbal Z, Bunn J, Lovel H, Harrington R. Impact of maternal depression on infant nutritional status and illness: a cohort study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61:946–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Pina-Camacho L, Jensen S, Gaysina D, Barker E. Maternal depression symptoms, unhealthy diet and child emotional–behavioural dysregulation. Psychol Med. 2015;45:1851–60.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Sohr-Preston SL, Scaramella LV. Implications of timing of maternal depressive symptoms for early cognitive and language development. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2006;9:65–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Wachs TD, Black MM, Engle PL. Maternal depression: a global threat to children’s health, development, and behavior and to human rights. Child Dev Perspect. 2009;3:51–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Rahman A, Fisher J, Bower P, Luchters S, Tran T, Yasamy MT, et al. Interventions for common perinatal mental disorders in women in low-and middle-income countries: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Bull World Health Organ. 2013;91:593–601I.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Walker SP, Wachs TD, Grantham-McGregor S, Black MM, Nelson CA, Huffman SL, et al. Inequality in early childhood: risk and protective factors for early child development. Lancet. 2011;378:1325–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Grantham-McGregor SM, Fernald LC, Kagawa R, Walker S. Effects of integrated child development and nutrition interventions on child development and nutritional status. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2014;1308:11–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Walker SP, Chang SM, Vera-Hernández M, Grantham-McGregor S. Early childhood stimulation benefits adult competence and reduces violent behavior. Pediatrics. 2011;127:849–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Engle PL, Fernald LC, Alderman H, Behrman J, O’Gara C, Yousafzai A, et al. Strategies for reducing inequalities and improving developmental outcomes for young children in low-income and middle-income countries. Lancet. 2011;378:1339–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Santos DN, Assis AMO, Bastos ACS, Santos LM, Santos CAS, Strina A, et al. Determinants of cognitive function in childhood: a cohort study in a middle income context. BMC Public Health. 2008;8:202.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Frost MB, Forste R, Haas DW. Maternal education and child nutritional status in Bolivia: finding the links. Soc Sci Med. 2005;60:395–407.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Semba RD, de Pee S, Sun K, Sari M, Akhter N, Bloem MW. Effect of parental formal education on risk of child stunting in Indonesia and Bangladesh: a cross-sectional study. Lancet. 2008;371:322–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. von der Lippe AL. The impact of maternal schooling and occupation on child-rearing attitudes and behaviours in low income neighbourhoods in Cairo, Egypt. Int J Behav Dev. 1999;23:703–29.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  57. Becker S, Fonseca-Becker F, Schenck-Yglesias C. Husbands’ and wives’ reports of women’s decision-making power in western Guatemala and their effects on preventive health behaviors. Soc Sci Med. 2006;62:2313–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Zhou X, Bi B, Zheng L, Li Z, Yang H, Song H, et al. The prevalence and risk factors for depression symptoms in a rural Chinese sample population. PLoS One. 2014;9:e99692.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  59. Akee R. Errors in self-reported earnings: the role of previous earnings volatility. Institute for the Study of labor discussion paper no 3263. 2007.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the local healthcare providers in the field work for data collection, and thank all the participants in this study.

Funding

UNICEF grants ‘Health and Nutrition’ (0315-China/YH702).

Availability of data and materials

The datasets analyzed during the current study are not publicly available, but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

CHZ and XYL did the data analysis. QWW and SSL prepared the background of early childhood development and inequality in children health. CHZ and CXZ drafted the article. SFG and RWS designed the study and collected the data. JXZ, XLW and XYL critically revised the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Jingxu Zhang or Xiaoli Wang.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was approved by Peking University’s Biomedical Ethics Committee. Consent was obtained from the participants/guardians/parents.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Additional file

Additional file 1:

Supplemental Tables Detailed information about sensitivity analyses. Table S1. Prevalence and concentration index of suspected developmental delay, with missing income imputed. Table S2. Associations of socioeconomic factors with suspected developmental delay, with missing income imputed. Table S3. Contributions of socioeconomic factors to inequality in suspected developmental delay, with missing income imputed. Table S4. Associations of socioeconomic factors with suspected developmental delay, with different cutoff points. Table S5. Concentration index of suspected developmental delay, with different cutoff points. Table S6. Contributions of socioeconomic factors to inequality in suspected developmental delay, with different cutoff points. (DOCX 25 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zhang, C., Zhao, C., Liu, X. et al. Inequality in early childhood neurodevelopment in six poor rural counties of China: a decomposition analysis. Int J Equity Health 16, 212 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12939-017-0691-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12939-017-0691-y

Keywords